Some animals have an uncanny ability to complete long distance migrations, and in many species that task is done repeatedly during each individual’s lifetime. Migrators are organisms that specialize in finding habitats ideal for particular activities at particular times during their growth and development. For example, they may occupy habitats as juveniles (“nursery” or “developmental” habitats), different from those occupied by adults. They may also use specific habitats for breeding, for overwintering, or for finding food or shelter.
All species of marine turtles are superb migrators! In fact, they demonstrate this ability almost immediately as hatchlings that have completed embryonic development within an underground nest. Hatchlings then dig their way upward to the beach surface, scamper unerringly toward the surf zone, and swim vigorously offshore and out to sea. Thus begins a migration toward oceanic nursery areas hundreds of miles distant where they usually reside for many years.
Figure 1. Offshore migration by hatchling sea turtles is accomplished by using three sets of cues in succession. After emerging from their nest, the turtles use visual cues to locate the surf zone, the motion of waves in shallow water to swim, and finally in deep water the earth’s magnetic field (where the waves no longer reliably indicate an offshore direction). (Modified from The Biology of Sea Turtles, Vol. I, CRC Press).
You might be curious about how a small turtle, minutes after it leaves an underground nest, can achieve a such a difficult task in a world it has never before experienced. Experiments done by inquisitive biologists have shown that the hatchlings accomplish this feat using three sets of cues, each of which keeps them on course during specific portions of their journey (Figure 1). While scampering down the beach, the turtles use visual cues; they crawl away from high structures (the dune and its vegetation toward land) and crawl toward lower vistas (the unobstructed view toward the sea). Once they enter the ocean, vision is switched
off and the turtles turn on a motion sensitivity that enables them to respond to the up-and-down displacements produced by oceanic waves where they swim: near the ocean surface. The turtles can actually use those displacements to determine the direction of the oncoming waves. They continue offshore by swimming in the opposite direction. This “wave compass” works because in shallow water, the waves are turned by bottom friction and always approach the shore parallel to the coastline (Figure 1). But later, in deeper water, wave direction is dictated by winds which can blow from other directions. Hatchlings “know” about how long their wave compass is reliable (~ 30 minutes) and so when that time ends, they switch to a third cue, the earth’s magnetic field, which dominates as a compass for the remainder of their migration. Magnetic cues also guide the migratory directions chosen by older turtles during their return from oceanic nursery sites to coastal habitats, where they finish growing. That return is characteristic of most species of marine turtles because food is more abundant near shore than in the open ocean, and more food results in faster, healthier growth.
Orientation biologists are fascinated by the precision and complexity of animal migratory movements as well as the variety of underlying mechanisms that enable the animals to complete these journeys. Here, I review their discoveries that explain how marine turtles migrate. In particular, I’ll emphasize the many experiments done by Professors Ken and Cathy Lohmann at the University of North Carolina, and by one of their doctoral students, Nathan Putman. It is largely through their efforts that we now understand how marine turtles became expert migrators, millions of years before we even thought about inventing the GPS!
After many years of study biologists realized that two processes, differing in complexity, appeared to control how all animals complete their migratory movements. One process is called orientation, or an ability to maintain a direction using an external cue, or guidepost, as a reference. Which cue is used varies with when and where the animal migrates; it can be astronomical in nature (the sun, stars, moon), an odor carried downstream by wind or water currents, a sound, the pull of gravity, the tides or, for sea turtles, even oceanic waves.